logo
Listen Language Learn
thumb

TED Talks - Most Popular - The puzzle of motivation

-
+
15
30

Career analyst Dan Pink examines the puzzle of motivation, starting with a fact that social scientists know but most managers don't: Traditional rewards aren't always as effective as we think. Listen for illuminating stories -- and maybe, a way forward.

I
need
to
make
a
confession
at
the
outset
here.
A
little
over
20
years
ago,
I
did
something
that
I
regret,
something
that
I'm
not
particularly
proud
of.
Something
that,
in
many
ways,
I
wish
no
one
would
ever
know,
but
here
I
feel
kind
of
obliged
to
reveal.
(Laughter)
In
the
late
1980s,
in
a
moment
of
youthful
indiscretion,
I
went
to
law
school.
(Laughter)
In
America,
law
is
a
professional
degree:
after
your
university
degree,
you
go
on
to
law
school.
When
I
got
to
law
school,
I
didn't
do
very
well.
To
put
it
mildly,
I
didn't
do
very
well.
I,
in
fact,
graduated
in
the
part
of
my
law
school
class
that
made
the
top
90%
possible.
(Laughter)
Thank
you.
I
never
practiced
law
a
day
in
my
life;
I
pretty
much
wasn't
allowed
to.
(Laughter)
But
today,
against
my
better
judgment,
against
the
advice
of
my
own
wife,
I
want
to
try
to
dust
off
some
of
those
legal
skills
--
what's
left
of
those
legal
skills.
I
don't
want
to
tell
you
a
story.
I
want
to
make
a
case.
I
want
to
make
a
hard-headed,
evidence-based,
dare
I
say
lawyerly
case,
for
rethinking
how
we
run
our
businesses.
So,
ladies
and
gentlemen
of
the
jury,
take
a
look
at
this.
This
is
called
the
candle
problem.
Some
of
you
might
know
it.
It's
created
in
1945
by
a
psychologist
named
Karl
Duncker.
He
created
this
experiment
that
is
used
in
many
other
experiments
in
behavioral
science.
And
here's
how
it
works.
Suppose
I'm
the
experimenter.
I
bring
you
into
a
room.
I
give
you
a
candle,
some
thumbtacks
and
some
matches.
And
I
say
to
you,
"Your
job
is
to
attach
the
candle
to
the
wall
so
the
wax
doesn't
drip
onto
the
table."
Now
what
would
you
do?
Many
people
begin
trying
to
thumbtack
the
candle
to
the
wall.
Doesn't
work.
I
saw
somebody
kind
of
make
the
motion
over
here
--
some
people
have
a
great
idea
where
they
light
the
match,
melt
the
side
of
the
candle,
try
to
adhere
it
to
the
wall.
It's
an
awesome
idea.
Doesn't
work.
And
eventually,
after
five
or
ten
minutes,
most
people
figure
out
the
solution,
which
you
can
see
here.
The
key
is
to
overcome
what's
called
functional
fixedness.
You
look
at
that
box
and
you
see
it
only
as
a
receptacle
for
the
tacks.
But
it
can
also
have
this
other
function,
as
a
platform
for
the
candle.
The
candle
problem.
I
want
to
tell
you
about
an
experiment
using
the
candle
problem,
done
by
a
scientist
named
Sam
Glucksberg,
who
is
now
at
Princeton
University,
US,
This
shows
the
power
of
incentives.
He
gathered
his
participants
and
said:
"I'm
going
to
time
you,
how
quickly
you
can
solve
this
problem."
To
one
group
he
said,
"I'm
going
to
time
you
to
establish
norms,
averages
for
how
long
it
typically
takes
someone
to
solve
this
sort
of
problem."
To
the
second
group
he
offered
rewards.
He
said,
"If
you're
in
the
top
25%
of
the
fastest
times,
you
get
five
dollars.
If
you're
the
fastest
of
everyone
we're
testing
here
today,
you
get
20
dollars."
Now
this
is
several
years
ago,
adjusted
for
inflation,
it's
a
decent
sum
of
money
for
a
few
minutes
of
work.
It's
a
nice
motivator.
Question:
How
much
faster
did
this
group
solve
the
problem?
Answer:
It
took
them,
on
average,
three
and
a
half
minutes
longer.
3.5
min
longer.
This
makes
no
sense,
right?
I
mean,
I'm
an
American.
I
believe
in
free
markets.
That's
not
how
it's
supposed
to
work,
right?
(Laughter)
If
you
want
people
to
perform
better,
you
reward
them.
Right?
Bonuses,
commissions,
their
own
reality
show.
Incentivize
them.
That's
how
business
works.
But
that's
not
happening
here.
You've
got
an
incentive
designed
to
sharpen
thinking
and
accelerate
creativity,
and
it
does
just
the
opposite.
It
dulls
thinking
and
blocks
creativity.
What's
interesting
about
this
experiment
is
that
it's
not
an
aberration.
This
has
been
replicated
over
and
over
again
for
nearly
40
years.
These
contingent
motivators
--
if
you
do
this,
then
you
get
that
--
work
in
some
circumstances.
But
for
a
lot
of
tasks,
they
actually
either
don't
work
or,
often,
they
do
harm.
This
is
one
of
the
most
robust
findings
in
social
science,
and
also
one
of
the
most
ignored.
I
spent
the
last
couple
of
years
looking
at
the
science
of
human
motivation,
particularly
the
dynamics
of
extrinsic
motivators
and
intrinsic
motivators.
And
I'm
telling
you,
it's
not
even
close.
If
you
look
at
the
science,
there
is
a
mismatch
between
what
science
knows
and
what
business
does.
What's
alarming
here
is
that
our
business
operating
system
--
think
of
the
set
of
assumptions
and
protocols
beneath
our
businesses,
how
we
motivate
people,
how
we
apply
our
human
resources--
it's
built
entirely
around
these
extrinsic
motivators,
around
carrots
and
sticks.
That's
actually
fine
for
many
kinds
of
20th
century
tasks.
But
for
21st
century
tasks,
that
mechanistic,
reward-and-punishment
approach
doesn't
work,
often
doesn't
work,
and
often
does
harm.
Let
me
show
you.
Glucksberg
did
another
similar
experiment,
he
presented
the
problem
in
a
slightly
different
way,
like
this
up
here.
Attach
the
candle
to
the
wall
so
the
wax
doesn't
drip
onto
the
table.
Same
deal.
You:
we're
timing
for
norms.
You:
we're
incentivizing.
What
happened
this
time?
This
time,
the
incentivized
group
kicked
the
other
group's
butt.
Why?
Because
when
the
tacks
are
out
of
the
box,
it's
pretty
easy
isn't
it?
(Laughter)
If-then
rewards
work
really
well
for
those
sorts
of
tasks,
where
there
is
a
simple
set
of
rules
and
a
clear
destination
to
go
to.
Rewards,
by
their
very
nature,
narrow
our
focus,
concentrate
the
mind;
that's
why
they
work
in
so
many
cases.
So,
for
tasks
like
this,
a
narrow
focus,
where
you
just
see
the
goal
right
there,
zoom
straight
ahead
to
it,
they
work
really
well.
But
for
the
real
candle
problem,
you
don't
want
to
be
looking
like
this.
The
solution
is
on
the
periphery.
You
want
to
be
looking
around.
That
reward
actually
narrows
our
focus
and
restricts
our
possibility.
Let
me
tell
you
why
this
is
so
important.
In
western
Europe,
in
many
parts
of
Asia,
in
North
America,
in
Australia,
white-collar
workers
are
doing
less
of
this
kind
of
work,
and
more
of
this
kind
of
work.
That
routine,
rule-based,
left-brain
work
--
certain
kinds
of
accounting,
financial
analysis,
computer
programming
--
has
become
fairly
easy
to
outsource,
fairly
easy
to
automate.
Software
can
do
it
faster.
Low-cost
providers
can
do
it
cheaper.
So
what
really
matters
are
the
more
right-brained
creative,
conceptual
kinds
of
abilities.
Think
about
your
own
work.
Think
about
your
own
work.
Are
the
problems
that
you
face,
or
even
the
problems
we've
been
talking
about
here,
do
they
have
a
clear
set
of
rules,
and
a
single
solution?
No.
The
rules
are
mystifying.
The
solution,
if
it
exists
at
all,
is
surprising
and
not
obvious.
Everybody
in
this
room
is
dealing
with
their
own
version
of
the
candle
problem.
And
for
candle
problems
of
any
kind,
in
any
field,
those
if-then
rewards,
the
things
around
which
we've
built
so
many
of
our
businesses,
don't
work!
It
makes
me
crazy.
And
here's
the
thing.
This
is
not
a
feeling.
Okay?
I'm
a
lawyer;
I
don't
believe
in
feelings.
This
is
not
a
philosophy.
I'm
an
American;
I
don't
believe
in
philosophy.
(Laughter)
This
is
a
fact
--
or,
as
we
say
in
my
hometown
of
Washington,
D.C.,
a
true
fact.
(Laughter)
(Applause)
Let
me
give
you
an
example.
Let
me
marshal
the
evidence
here.
I'm
not
telling
a
story,
I'm
making
a
case.
Ladies
and
gentlemen
of
the
jury,
some
evidence:
Dan
Ariely,
one
of
the
great
economists
of
our
time,
he
and
three
colleagues
did
a
study
of
some
MIT
students.
They
gave
these
MIT
students
a
bunch
of
games,
games
that
involved
creativity,
and
motor
skills,
and
concentration.
And
the
offered
them,
for
performance,
three
levels
of
rewards:
small
reward,
medium
reward,
large
reward.
If
you
do
really
well
you
get
the
large
reward,
on
down.
What
happened?
As
long
as
the
task
involved
only
mechanical
skill
bonuses
worked
as
they
would
be
expected:
the
higher
the
pay,
the
better
the
performance.
Okay?
But
once
the
task
called
for
even
rudimentary
cognitive
skill,
a
larger
reward
led
to
poorer
performance.
Then
they
said,
"Let's
see
if
there's
any
cultural
bias
here.
Let's
go
to
Madurai,
India
and
test
it."
Standard
of
living
is
lower.
In
Madurai,
a
reward
that
is
modest
in
North
American
standards,
is
more
meaningful
there.
Same
deal.
A
bunch
of
games,
three
levels
of
rewards.
What
happens?
People
offered
the
medium
level
of
rewards
did
no
better
than
people
offered
the
small
rewards.
But
this
time,
people
offered
the
highest
rewards,
they
did
the
worst
of
all.
In
eight
of
the
nine
tasks
we
examined
across
three
experiments,
higher
incentives
led
to
worse
performance.
Is
this
some
kind
of
touchy-feely
socialist
conspiracy
going
on
here?
No,
these
are
economists
from
MIT,
from
Carnegie
Mellon,
from
the
University
of
Chicago.
Do
you
know
who
sponsored
this
research?
The
Federal
Reserve
Bank
of
the
United
States.
That's
the
American
experience.
Let's
go
across
the
pond
to
the
London
School
of
Economics,
LSE,
London
School
of
Economics,
alma
mater
of
eleven
Nobel
Laureates
in
economics.
Training
ground
for
great
economic
thinkers
like
George
Soros,
and
Friedrich
Hayek,
and
Mick
Jagger.
(Laughter)
Last
month,
just
last
month,
economists
at
LSE
looked
at
51
studies
of
pay-for-performance
plans,
inside
of
companies.
Here's
what
they
said:
"We
find
that
financial
incentives
can
result
in
a
negative
impact
on
overall
performance."
There
is
a
mismatch
between
what
science
knows
and
what
business
does.
And
what
worries
me,
as
we
stand
here
in
the
rubble
of
the
economic
collapse,
is
that
too
many
organizations
are
making
their
decisions,
their
policies
about
talent
and
people,
based
on
assumptions
that
are
outdated,
unexamined,
and
rooted
more
in
folklore
than
in
science.
And
if
we
really
want
to
get
out
of
this
economic
mess,
if
we
really
want
high
performance
on
those
definitional
tasks
of
the
21st
century,
the
solution
is
not
to
do
more
of
the
wrong
things,
to
entice
people
with
a
sweeter
carrot,
or
threaten
them
with
a
sharper
stick.
We
need
a
whole
new
approach.
The
good
news
is
that
the
scientists
who've
been
studying
motivation
have
given
us
this
new
approach.
It's
built
much
more
around
intrinsic
motivation.
Around
the
desire
to
do
things
because
they
matter,
because
we
like
it,
they're
interesting,
or
part
of
something
important.
And
to
my
mind,
that
new
operating
system
for
our
businesses
revolves
around
three
elements:
autonomy,
mastery
and
purpose.
Autonomy:
the
urge
to
direct
our
own
lives.
Mastery:
the
desire
to
get
better
and
better
at
something
that
matters.
Purpose:
the
yearning
to
do
what
we
do
in
the
service
of
something
larger
than
ourselves.
These
are
the
building
blocks
of
an
entirely
new
operating
system
for
our
businesses.
I
want
to
talk
today
only
about
autonomy.
In
the
20th
century,
we
came
up
with
this
idea
of
management.
Management
did
not
emanate
from
nature.
Management
is
not
a
tree,
it's
a
television
set.
Somebody
invented
it.
It
doesn't
mean
it's
going
to
work
forever.
Management
is
great.
Traditional
notions
of
management
are
great
if
you
want
compliance.
But
if
you
want
engagement,
self-direction
works
better.
Some
examples
of
some
kind
of
radical
notions
of
self-direction.
You
don't
see
a
lot
of
it,
but
you
see
the
first
stirrings
of
something
really
interesting
going
on,
what
it
means
is
paying
people
adequately
and
fairly,
absolutely
--
getting
the
issue
of
money
off
the
table,
and
then
giving
people
lots
of
autonomy.
Some
examples.
How
many
of
you
have
heard
of
the
company
Atlassian?
It
looks
like
less
than
half.
(Laughter)
Atlassian
is
an
Australian
software
company.
And
they
do
something
incredibly
cool.
A
few
times
a
year
they
tell
their
engineers,
"Go
for
the
next
24
hours
and
work
on
anything
you
want,
as
long
as
it's
not
part
of
your
regular
job.
Work
on
anything
you
want."
Engineers
use
this
time
to
come
up
with
a
cool
patch
for
code,
come
up
with
an
elegant
hack.
Then
they
present
all
of
the
stuff
that
they've
developed
to
their
teammates,
to
the
rest
of
the
company,
in
this
wild
and
woolly
all-hands
meeting
at
the
end
of
the
day.
Being
Australians,
everybody
has
a
beer.
They
call
them
FedEx
Days.
Why?
Because
you
have
to
deliver
something
overnight.
It's
pretty;
not
bad.
It's
a
huge
trademark
violation,
but
it's
pretty
clever.
(Laughter)
That
one
day
of
intense
autonomy
has
produced
a
whole
array
of
software
fixes
that
might
never
have
existed.
It's
worked
so
well
that
Atlassian
has
taken
it
to
the
next
level
with
20%
time
--
done,
famously,
at
Google
--
where
engineers
can
spend
20%
of
their
time
working
on
anything
they
want.
They
have
autonomy
over
their
time,
their
task,
their
team,
their
technique.
Radical
amounts
of
autonomy.
And
at
Google,
as
many
of
you
know,
about
half
of
the
new
products
in
a
typical
year
are
birthed
during
that
20%
time:
things
like
Gmail,
Orkut,
Google
News.
Let
me
give
you
an
even
more
radical
example
of
it:
something
called
the
Results
Only
Work
Environment
(the
ROWE),
created
by
two
American
consultants,
in
place
at
a
dozen
companies
around
North
America.
In
a
ROWE
people
don't
have
schedules.
They
show
up
when
they
want.
They
don't
have
to
be
in
the
office
at
a
certain
time,
or
any
time.
They
just
have
to
get
their
work
done.
How
they
do
it,
when
they
do
it,
where
they
do
it,
is
totally
up
to
them.
Meetings
in
these
kinds
of
environments
are
optional.
What
happens?
Almost
across
the
board,
productivity
goes
up,
worker
engagement
goes
up,
worker
satisfaction
goes
up,
turnover
goes
down.
Autonomy,
mastery
and
purpose,
the
building
blocks
of
a
new
way
of
doing
things.
Some
of
you
might
look
at
this
and
say,
"Hmm,
that
sounds
nice,
but
it's
Utopian."
And
I
say,
"Nope.
I
have
proof."
The
mid-1990s,
Microsoft
started
an
encyclopedia
called
Encarta.
They
had
deployed
all
the
right
incentives,
They
paid
professionals
to
write
and
edit
thousands
of
articles.
Well-compensated
managers
oversaw
the
whole
thing
to
make
sure
it
came
in
on
budget
and
on
time.
A
few
years
later,
another
encyclopedia
got
started.
Different
model,
right?
Do
it
for
fun.
No
one
gets
paid
a
cent,
or
a
euro
or
a
yen.
Do
it
because
you
like
to
do
it.
Just
10
years
ago,
if
you
had
gone
to
an
economist,
anywhere,
"Hey,
I've
got
these
two
different
models
for
creating
an
encyclopedia.
If
they
went
head
to
head,
who
would
win?"
10
years
ago
you
could
not
have
found
a
single
sober
economist
anywhere
on
planet
Earth
who
would
have
predicted
the
Wikipedia
model.
This
is
the
titanic
battle
between
these
two
approaches.
This
is
the
Ali-Frazier
of
motivation,
right?
This
is
the
Thrilla
in
Manila.
Intrinsic
motivators
versus
extrinsic
motivators.
Autonomy,
mastery
and
purpose,
versus
carrot
and
sticks,
and
who
wins?
Intrinsic
motivation,
autonomy,
mastery
and
purpose,
in
a
knockout.
Let
me
wrap
up.
There
is
a
mismatch
between
what
science
knows
and
what
business
does.
Here
is
what
science
knows.
One:
Those
20th
century
rewards,
those
motivators
we
think
are
a
natural
part
of
business,
do
work,
but
only
in
a
surprisingly
narrow
band
of
circumstances.
Two:
Those
if-then
rewards
often
destroy
creativity.
Three:
The
secret
to
high
performance
isn't
rewards
and
punishments,
but
that
unseen
intrinsic
drive--
the
drive
to
do
things
for
their
own
sake.
The
drive
to
do
things
cause
they
matter.
And
here's
the
best
part.
We
already
know
this.
The
science
confirms
what
we
know
in
our
hearts.
So,
if
we
repair
this
mismatch
between
science
and
business,
if
we
bring
our
motivation,
notions
of
motivation
into
the
21st
century,
if
we
get
past
this
lazy,
dangerous,
ideology
of
carrots
and
sticks,
we
can
strengthen
our
businesses,
we
can
solve
a
lot
of
those
candle
problems,
and
maybe,
maybe
--
we
can
change
the
world.
I
rest
my
case.
(Applause)
Check out more TED Talks - Most Popular

See below for the full transcript

I need to make a confession at the outset here. A little over 20 years ago, I did something that I regret, something that I'm not particularly proud of. Something that, in many ways, I wish no one would ever know, but here I feel kind of obliged to reveal. (Laughter) In the late 1980s, in a moment of youthful indiscretion, I went to law school. (Laughter) In America, law is a professional degree: after your university degree, you go on to law school. When I got to law school, I didn't do very well. To put it mildly, I didn't do very well. I, in fact, graduated in the part of my law school class that made the top 90% possible. (Laughter) Thank you. I never practiced law a day in my life; I pretty much wasn't allowed to. (Laughter) But today, against my better judgment, against the advice of my own wife, I want to try to dust off some of those legal skills -- what's left of those legal skills. I don't want to tell you a story. I want to make a case. I want to make a hard-headed, evidence-based, dare I say lawyerly case, for rethinking how we run our businesses. So, ladies and gentlemen of the jury, take a look at this. This is called the candle problem. Some of you might know it. It's created in 1945 by a psychologist named Karl Duncker. He created this experiment that is used in many other experiments in behavioral science. And here's how it works. Suppose I'm the experimenter. I bring you into a room. I give you a candle, some thumbtacks and some matches. And I say to you, "Your job is to attach the candle to the wall so the wax doesn't drip onto the table." Now what would you do? Many people begin trying to thumbtack the candle to the wall. Doesn't work. I saw somebody kind of make the motion over here -- some people have a great idea where they light the match, melt the side of the candle, try to adhere it to the wall. It's an awesome idea. Doesn't work. And eventually, after five or ten minutes, most people figure out the solution, which you can see here. The key is to overcome what's called functional fixedness. You look at that box and you see it only as a receptacle for the tacks. But it can also have this other function, as a platform for the candle. The candle problem. I want to tell you about an experiment using the candle problem, done by a scientist named Sam Glucksberg, who is now at Princeton University, US, This shows the power of incentives. He gathered his participants and said: "I'm going to time you, how quickly you can solve this problem." To one group he said, "I'm going to time you to establish norms, averages for how long it typically takes someone to solve this sort of problem." To the second group he offered rewards. He said, "If you're in the top 25% of the fastest times, you get five dollars. If you're the fastest of everyone we're testing here today, you get 20 dollars." Now this is several years ago, adjusted for inflation, it's a decent sum of money for a few minutes of work. It's a nice motivator. Question: How much faster did this group solve the problem? Answer: It took them, on average, three and a half minutes longer. 3.5 min longer. This makes no sense, right? I mean, I'm an American. I believe in free markets. That's not how it's supposed to work, right? (Laughter) If you want people to perform better, you reward them. Right? Bonuses, commissions, their own reality show. Incentivize them. That's how business works. But that's not happening here. You've got an incentive designed to sharpen thinking and accelerate creativity, and it does just the opposite. It dulls thinking and blocks creativity. What's interesting about this experiment is that it's not an aberration. This has been replicated over and over again for nearly 40 years. These contingent motivators -- if you do this, then you get that -- work in some circumstances. But for a lot of tasks, they actually either don't work or, often, they do harm. This is one of the most robust findings in social science, and also one of the most ignored. I spent the last couple of years looking at the science of human motivation, particularly the dynamics of extrinsic motivators and intrinsic motivators. And I'm telling you, it's not even close. If you look at the science, there is a mismatch between what science knows and what business does. What's alarming here is that our business operating system -- think of the set of assumptions and protocols beneath our businesses, how we motivate people, how we apply our human resources-- it's built entirely around these extrinsic motivators, around carrots and sticks. That's actually fine for many kinds of 20th century tasks. But for 21st century tasks, that mechanistic, reward-and-punishment approach doesn't work, often doesn't work, and often does harm. Let me show you. Glucksberg did another similar experiment, he presented the problem in a slightly different way, like this up here. Attach the candle to the wall so the wax doesn't drip onto the table. Same deal. You: we're timing for norms. You: we're incentivizing. What happened this time? This time, the incentivized group kicked the other group's butt. Why? Because when the tacks are out of the box, it's pretty easy isn't it? (Laughter) If-then rewards work really well for those sorts of tasks, where there is a simple set of rules and a clear destination to go to. Rewards, by their very nature, narrow our focus, concentrate the mind; that's why they work in so many cases. So, for tasks like this, a narrow focus, where you just see the goal right there, zoom straight ahead to it, they work really well. But for the real candle problem, you don't want to be looking like this. The solution is on the periphery. You want to be looking around. That reward actually narrows our focus and restricts our possibility. Let me tell you why this is so important. In western Europe, in many parts of Asia, in North America, in Australia, white-collar workers are doing less of this kind of work, and more of this kind of work. That routine, rule-based, left-brain work -- certain kinds of accounting, financial analysis, computer programming -- has become fairly easy to outsource, fairly easy to automate. Software can do it faster. Low-cost providers can do it cheaper. So what really matters are the more right-brained creative, conceptual kinds of abilities. Think about your own work. Think about your own work. Are the problems that you face, or even the problems we've been talking about here, do they have a clear set of rules, and a single solution? No. The rules are mystifying. The solution, if it exists at all, is surprising and not obvious. Everybody in this room is dealing with their own version of the candle problem. And for candle problems of any kind, in any field, those if-then rewards, the things around which we've built so many of our businesses, don't work! It makes me crazy. And here's the thing. This is not a feeling. Okay? I'm a lawyer; I don't believe in feelings. This is not a philosophy. I'm an American; I don't believe in philosophy. (Laughter) This is a fact -- or, as we say in my hometown of Washington, D.C., a true fact. (Laughter) (Applause) Let me give you an example. Let me marshal the evidence here. I'm not telling a story, I'm making a case. Ladies and gentlemen of the jury, some evidence: Dan Ariely, one of the great economists of our time, he and three colleagues did a study of some MIT students. They gave these MIT students a bunch of games, games that involved creativity, and motor skills, and concentration. And the offered them, for performance, three levels of rewards: small reward, medium reward, large reward. If you do really well you get the large reward, on down. What happened? As long as the task involved only mechanical skill bonuses worked as they would be expected: the higher the pay, the better the performance. Okay? But once the task called for even rudimentary cognitive skill, a larger reward led to poorer performance. Then they said, "Let's see if there's any cultural bias here. Let's go to Madurai, India and test it." Standard of living is lower. In Madurai, a reward that is modest in North American standards, is more meaningful there. Same deal. A bunch of games, three levels of rewards. What happens? People offered the medium level of rewards did no better than people offered the small rewards. But this time, people offered the highest rewards, they did the worst of all. In eight of the nine tasks we examined across three experiments, higher incentives led to worse performance. Is this some kind of touchy-feely socialist conspiracy going on here? No, these are economists from MIT, from Carnegie Mellon, from the University of Chicago. Do you know who sponsored this research? The Federal Reserve Bank of the United States. That's the American experience. Let's go across the pond to the London School of Economics, LSE, London School of Economics, alma mater of eleven Nobel Laureates in economics. Training ground for great economic thinkers like George Soros, and Friedrich Hayek, and Mick Jagger. (Laughter) Last month, just last month, economists at LSE looked at 51 studies of pay-for-performance plans, inside of companies. Here's what they said: "We find that financial incentives can result in a negative impact on overall performance." There is a mismatch between what science knows and what business does. And what worries me, as we stand here in the rubble of the economic collapse, is that too many organizations are making their decisions, their policies about talent and people, based on assumptions that are outdated, unexamined, and rooted more in folklore than in science. And if we really want to get out of this economic mess, if we really want high performance on those definitional tasks of the 21st century, the solution is not to do more of the wrong things, to entice people with a sweeter carrot, or threaten them with a sharper stick. We need a whole new approach. The good news is that the scientists who've been studying motivation have given us this new approach. It's built much more around intrinsic motivation. Around the desire to do things because they matter, because we like it, they're interesting, or part of something important. And to my mind, that new operating system for our businesses revolves around three elements: autonomy, mastery and purpose. Autonomy: the urge to direct our own lives. Mastery: the desire to get better and better at something that matters. Purpose: the yearning to do what we do in the service of something larger than ourselves. These are the building blocks of an entirely new operating system for our businesses. I want to talk today only about autonomy. In the 20th century, we came up with this idea of management. Management did not emanate from nature. Management is not a tree, it's a television set. Somebody invented it. It doesn't mean it's going to work forever. Management is great. Traditional notions of management are great if you want compliance. But if you want engagement, self-direction works better. Some examples of some kind of radical notions of self-direction. You don't see a lot of it, but you see the first stirrings of something really interesting going on, what it means is paying people adequately and fairly, absolutely -- getting the issue of money off the table, and then giving people lots of autonomy. Some examples. How many of you have heard of the company Atlassian? It looks like less than half. (Laughter) Atlassian is an Australian software company. And they do something incredibly cool. A few times a year they tell their engineers, "Go for the next 24 hours and work on anything you want, as long as it's not part of your regular job. Work on anything you want." Engineers use this time to come up with a cool patch for code, come up with an elegant hack. Then they present all of the stuff that they've developed to their teammates, to the rest of the company, in this wild and woolly all-hands meeting at the end of the day. Being Australians, everybody has a beer. They call them FedEx Days. Why? Because you have to deliver something overnight. It's pretty; not bad. It's a huge trademark violation, but it's pretty clever. (Laughter) That one day of intense autonomy has produced a whole array of software fixes that might never have existed. It's worked so well that Atlassian has taken it to the next level with 20% time -- done, famously, at Google -- where engineers can spend 20% of their time working on anything they want. They have autonomy over their time, their task, their team, their technique. Radical amounts of autonomy. And at Google, as many of you know, about half of the new products in a typical year are birthed during that 20% time: things like Gmail, Orkut, Google News. Let me give you an even more radical example of it: something called the Results Only Work Environment (the ROWE), created by two American consultants, in place at a dozen companies around North America. In a ROWE people don't have schedules. They show up when they want. They don't have to be in the office at a certain time, or any time. They just have to get their work done. How they do it, when they do it, where they do it, is totally up to them. Meetings in these kinds of environments are optional. What happens? Almost across the board, productivity goes up, worker engagement goes up, worker satisfaction goes up, turnover goes down. Autonomy, mastery and purpose, the building blocks of a new way of doing things. Some of you might look at this and say, "Hmm, that sounds nice, but it's Utopian." And I say, "Nope. I have proof." The mid-1990s, Microsoft started an encyclopedia called Encarta. They had deployed all the right incentives, They paid professionals to write and edit thousands of articles. Well-compensated managers oversaw the whole thing to make sure it came in on budget and on time. A few years later, another encyclopedia got started. Different model, right? Do it for fun. No one gets paid a cent, or a euro or a yen. Do it because you like to do it. Just 10 years ago, if you had gone to an economist, anywhere, "Hey, I've got these two different models for creating an encyclopedia. If they went head to head, who would win?" 10 years ago you could not have found a single sober economist anywhere on planet Earth who would have predicted the Wikipedia model. This is the titanic battle between these two approaches. This is the Ali-Frazier of motivation, right? This is the Thrilla in Manila. Intrinsic motivators versus extrinsic motivators. Autonomy, mastery and purpose, versus carrot and sticks, and who wins? Intrinsic motivation, autonomy, mastery and purpose, in a knockout. Let me wrap up. There is a mismatch between what science knows and what business does. Here is what science knows. One: Those 20th century rewards, those motivators we think are a natural part of business, do work, but only in a surprisingly narrow band of circumstances. Two: Those if-then rewards often destroy creativity. Three: The secret to high performance isn't rewards and punishments, but that unseen intrinsic drive-- the drive to do things for their own sake. The drive to do things cause they matter. And here's the best part. We already know this. The science confirms what we know in our hearts. So, if we repair this mismatch between science and business, if we bring our motivation, notions of motivation into the 21st century, if we get past this lazy, dangerous, ideology of carrots and sticks, we can strengthen our businesses, we can solve a lot of those candle problems, and maybe, maybe -- we can change the world. I rest my case. (Applause)

Translation Word Bank
AdBlock detected!

Your Add Blocker will interfere with the Google Translator. Please disable it for a better experience.

dismiss